Journal of Threatened Taxa | www.threatenedtaxa.org | 26 November 2016 | 8(13): 9512–9524

 

275477.jpg

A comparison of the effectiveness of methods of deterring pteropodid bats from feeding on commercial fruit in Madagascar

 

Tatamo E.A. Raharimihaja 1, Jo L.M. Rakotoarison 2, Paul A. Racey 3 & Radosoa A. Andrianaivoarivelo 4

 

1,2,4 Department of Animal Biology, Faculty of Sciences, University of Antananarivo, BP 906, Antananarivo 101, Madagascar

3 Centre for Ecology and Conservation, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter,

Cornwall Campus, TR10 9FE, UK

4 Aro Velona, Lot C 084 D Antovontany, Ambohijanaka, Antananarivo Atsimondrano 103, Madagascar

1 nastiatatamo@yahoo.fr, 2 rakotoarison.laurent@gmail.com, 3 p.a.racey@exeter.ac.uk (corresponding author), 4 aniainodna@yahoo.fr

 

 

 

doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.11609/jott.2688.8.13.9512-9524

 

Editor: H. Raghuram, The American College, Madurai, India. Date of publication: 26 November 2016 (online & print)

Manuscript details: Ms # 2688 | Received 20 April 2016 | Final received 05 November 2016 | Finally accepted 09 November 2016

Citation: Raharimihaja, T.E.A., J.L.M. Rakotoarison, P.A. Racey & R.A. Andrianaivoarivelo (2016). A comparison of the effectiveness of methods of deterring pteropodid bats from feeding on commercial fruit in Madagascar. Journal of Threatened Taxa 8(13): 9512–9524; http://dx.doi.org/10.11609/jott.2688.8.13.9512-9524

Copyright: © Raharimihaja et al. 2016. Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. JoTT allows unrestricted use of this article in any medium, reproduction and distribution by providing adequate credit to the authors and the source of publication.

Funding: International Foundation for Science.

Conflict of Interest: The authors declare no competing interests.

Author Details: Tatamo Eugenie Andréa Raharimihaja is a conservation biologist with a Master’s. degree from the University of Antananarivo on the effect of forest edges on Verreauxi’s Sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi) in Kirindy forest, western Madagascar. She is now a consultant in the Department of Zoology, University of Antananarivo, investigating conflicts between biodiversity and human society. This article is her first publication. Jo Laurent Mahery Rakotoarison also has a Master’s degree from the University of Antananarivo. Presently, he is a field biologist with the organization Malagasy Miaro Wild which works mainly on protection and conservation of lemurs in the Anjanaharibe-Sud Special Reserve. His main area of interest is the conservation of threatened Malagasy vertebrates and their unique habitats. Paul A Racey is Regius Professor of Natural History (Emeritus), University of Aberdeen and Honorary Visiting Professor at the University of Exeter in Cornwall. He has worked on Malagasy mammals for 30 years, mainly on bats, and helped to establish the organization Madagasikara Voakajy. Radosoa A. Andrianaivoarivelo obtained his Doctorate in Biology jointly from the Universities of Rennes1 and Antananarivo for research on the ecology and population biology of Madagascar’s smallest fruit bat Rousettus madagascariensis. Presently Rado and his team attempt to understand local community needs and carry out conservation action when natural habitats are threatened, in particular attempting to mitigate unsustainable exploitation of natural resources.

Author Contributions: TEAR collected field data at both study sites, analysed it using R-statistics and wrote the first draft of the report to the funding agency. JLMR was also involved in data collection at both study sites and used it for his Master’s thesis. PAR edited the original research report into a journal paper, coordinated the improvements to successive drafts and steered the paper through the publication process. RAA was project manager for the research. He successfully sought funding and contacted the stakeholders involved during the study. He assisted and supervised the team in the field, advised and taught the statistical methods used in the paper. He commented and improved on previous versions of this manuscript.

Acknowledgements: The Better Life for Children Association kindly provided transport to and from Ampasimaneva and accommodation there. We thank Idea Wild for a high resolution Canon digital camera. Julie Pomerantz and  Peter Balasky kindly provided the Plantskydd®. We thank the teachers of the Ampasimaneva College for assistance with fieldwork in Ampasimaneva (Anosibe An’Ala). We also thank Félix Rakotondraparany, former Head of the Animal Biology Department at the University of Antananarivo for support and the Ministry of Environment, Forest and Tourism for permission to work in the two districts, the COBA members (Communauté de Base) of the Amborabao Village (Tolagnaro) led by Florent Maka, who helped us during the data collection. Through this research project, Seheno Rasoanomenjanahary and Misa Ravalimahery (Department of Animal Biology, University of Antananarivo) helped us during the data collection in Ampasimaneva and Tolagnaro.

 

 

 

 

Abstract: We compared the effectiveness of methods of deterring Pteropus rufus from feeding on commercial fruit in east central and southeastern Madagascar in 20122013 during the Litchi chinensis harvest. Two of the three methods used, installing plastic flags and ringing bells in the trees, were derived from those used by litchi growers in the southeast. We improved and standardized these methods and compared their effectiveness with an organic product made from dried blood and vegetable oil (Plantskydd®) with a taste and odour aimed at deterring mammal feeding. The bats damaged from 4407,040 g of litchi fruits per tree and two of the three methods reduced the fruit lost to bats: the plastic flags and the organic deterrent. There were significant differences in the damage levels between the study sites and between our three methods of deterrence. The plastic flags and bell ringing methods were significantly less effective in reducing the fruit bat damage compared to the taste deterrent. The latter was most effective when it had enough time to dry and adhere to the fruits after spraying and before rain. Its effectiveness was further demonstrated in flight cage experiments during which Rousettus madagascariensis avoided litchis treated with Plantskydd®. Analysis of bat faecal samples revealed no feeding preference but the collected samples contained large numbers of Ficus seeds, suggesting that the bats feed extensively on Ficus fruits rather than on fruit of economic importance. Apart from fruit ripeness, tree productivity or other phenological factors did not affect the amount of fruit eaten by the bats. More fruits were damaged by birds than bats at both study sites.

 

Keywords: Bat-human conflict, bells, deterrents, litchi, Plantskydd®, plastic flags, Pteropus.

 

 

 

 

INTRODUCTION

As a result of habitat destruction and associated loss of the native plant species which comprised the diet of Old World fruit bats, there is growing evidence that exotic plants have become an important food resource for these bats (Aziz et al. 2016). In Madagascar, the Flying Fox Pteropus rufus, has a wide diet including some economically important introduced species such as Tamarindus indica leaves (Raheriarisena 2005), Agave sisalana pollen and nectar (Long & Racey 2007), Dimocarpus longan (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2007) and Litchi chinensis fruits (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2012). The Madagascar Rousette Rousettus madagascariensis also feeds on litchi fruits and banana (Goodman 1999), and the straw-coloured fruit bat Eidolon dupreanum includes guavas and passion fruit in its diet (Picot et al. 2007).

As a result, these three Madagascar fruit bat species, all endemics, are considered to be pests by some litchi growers because they feed on ripe fruit. Litchis are also eaten by birds, although there has been no comparison of the loss to different pest species. Aziz et al. (2016) reviewed methods of mitigating damage by fruit bats and concluded that netting was the only effective method of protecting fruit. Although lights, aversive smells and tastes have been used to deter bats, there have been no systematic tests of their effectiveness.

Litchis were introduced to Madagascar by 1802 and grow well in the southeastern and eastern parts of the island which has become the world’s third largest litchi producer (Menzel 2002). Litchis contribute significantly to improving the income of many Malagasy and losses to pests decreases that income. The primary aim of our study was to compare visual, sonic and biological deterrents which might be used to reduce the impact of bats on litchi fruit. In particular, we tested the hypothesis that an aversive taste deterrent, widely used in the northern hemisphere to discourage grazing and browsing herbivores would deter P. rufus from feeding on litchis. We also predicted that bats would feed on taller and more productive litchi trees. Our work also provided an opportunity to compare the damage to fruits by bats and other vertebrates.

 

 

METHODS

 

Study sites

The study was conducted during 2012 and 2013 at two widely separated sites (Image 1). Site 1 was at Amborabao Village in Tolagnaro District (24°19’30.8”S, 47°07’16.5”E, Commune Rurale Mahatalaky, Anosy Region, southeastern Madagascar), where up to 50 litchi trees occurred 2km from a P. rufus roost inside a small forest fragment: a sacred place called Kibory situated to the east of the village. This site is close to the large forest fragment: the Tsitongambarika Reserve that is managed jointly by the local communities (COBAs) and the Asity Organization. Site 2 was in eastcentral Madagascar at Ampasimaneva (19023’00”S & 48020’00.0”E, Anosibe An’ala District, Alaotra Mangoro Region). This region is characterized by scattered forest fragments with mid-altitude humid climate (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2007) and includes a roost of P. rufus. The fruiting season of litchis at Site 2 was two to three weeks later than at Site 1. The straight-line distance separating the two sites is 607km.

 

308774.jpg

 

 

Litchi productivity

We measured the crown diameter as the furthest distance between the two extremities in the horizontal plane for every litchi tree studied. Tree height was calculated by trigonometry. The GPS coordinates for each sampled litchi tree were also recorded. Total fruit productivity per tree was estimated by counting the number of fruits in one panicle and multiplying that by the total number of fruit-bearing panicles. This method was a development of that proposed by Sud et al. (2015). Although we reduced potential observer bias largely through maintaining the same survey team, it was still difficult to count the litchi fruits because some may have remained hidden from view. To allow for this, counts were repeated nine times by three observers, and we established that about 30% of the observed fruits could not be seen and counted. To correct for our underestimate, we added 30% to the total number of litchi fruits subsequently counted.

 

Methods of deterring fruit bats from feeding on litchis

We used three different methods of deterring fruit bats from feeding on litchis and tested the success of each. Two of these methods were inspired by those previously used by litchi farmers especially at Site 1 and were improved and standardized. The farmers at Site 2 used no deterrent apart from waving a long wooden stick to kill R. madagascariensis (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2007), which fed on their longan trees (D. longan).

 

(a) Visual Method: plastic flags

In 2007, at Site 1, at least 10 households were seen using coloured plastic flags attached near the most clustered ripe fruits to scare bats attempting to land on litchi branches. We standardized the size (1m x 0.5m) and colour of the plastic flags and selected branches carrying a similar number (100–150) of litchi fruits. The clapping sounds produced by the plastic flags blowing in the wind and their bright pink colour contrasted with the background green leaves (Musyoki 2014). We selected 37 focal fruit groups and randomized the samples observed during the study (Image 2). A fruit group consists of four to 10 panicles grouped in the same place, randomly chosen in a tree as a sample unit.

 

(b) Sonic method: bells

A bell 12cm in diameter at the open end was attached to the branch approximately 1m from the focal groups of litchis. We rang the bell by means of a string, with one end attached to the bell and the other pulled down by the observer (Image 3). When a flying fox was seen flying around the litchi fruit groups, we allowed the bat to land and approach the fruits and then rang the bell up to six times (with a five second interval between consecutive rings) to disturb the bat attempting to feed on the litchi. We stopped ringing the bell if the bat did not take off after six bell rings. We used six bells, four in one tree and two in another and we changed their places three times. The bell was left in place on the branch for four consecutive nights. Bell ringing sessions were conducted after sunset from 18:00hr until 22:00hr.

 

314041.jpg

 

 

(c) Biological control: Plantskydd®

Plantskydd® is a water-soluble powder which deters some mammals such as rabbits or deer from feeding on crops (Wagner et al. 2001). The active ingredient is food-grade dried blood (porcine or bovine) with vegetable oil added by the manufacturers to act as a binder to the crop being protected. Plantskydd® works by emitting an odour that repels animals before they eat the plant. We tested the effectiveness of Plantskydd® to control bat damage to litchis. The Plantskydd® adheres to the exocarp of the litchi and does not permeate it so does not affect the taste of the fruit.

A kilogram of Plantskydd® was added to 18.6 litres of water. We sprayed the resulting solution onto the target litchi fruit groups with a garden sprayer attached on a long wooden stick (Image 4a). A total of 46 fruit groups with one to two fruit groups per tree were treated (Image 4b). Each group contained 60 to 125 fruits and the Plantskydd® was applied when the fruits were at a green-red unripe status (around 15 days before they became ripe). If the solution is allowed to dry for six hours after the application it is considered by the manufacturers to be effective for to up to six months in summer even after heavy rainfall (http://www.plantskydd.com).

We used 53 fruit groups or samples as controls, with 21 at Site 1 and 32 at Site 2 where the deterrence methods were on trial.

 

Damage to litchis

To quantify the level of damage to the fruit, we monitored the litchis early each day (from 06:00hr to 09:00hr), from when they were ripe until they were all collected by the farmers and no more were left on the sampled trees. Fruits that were considered damaged by animals (Image 5) were subdivided into four groups using the following criteria:

Bats: distinctive teeth marks, which were larger for P. rufus or E. dupreanum and smaller for R. madagascariensis. Bat teeth clearly showed unambiguous dual canine punctures (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2012), the distance between the puncture corresponding to the distance between the canine tips of each of the three species.

Birds: distinctive beak marks of Acridothere tristis or Coracopsis spp.

Other mammals: incisor marks of rats.

Humans: the exocarp was peeled by humans and left beneath the tree.

 

Faecal analyses

Faecal samples were collected using plastic sheeting (2 or 3m x1m) placed beneath the roost trees during the day (Stashko & Dinerstein 1988) (Image 6a). To prevent consumption or removal of pellets by terrestrial vertebrates, they were set 1m above the ground (Tang et al. 2007). We checked the roosts twice a week and collected the faecal samples which were then sun dried and stored in small paper envelopes for later identification of seeds. At our two sites, we obtained a total of 277 items (seeds, fibre, vegetable debris, and unidentifiable viscous items) in 126 faecal samples from P. rufus during the study, 54 from site 1 and 72 from site 2 (Image 6b).

 

Identification of seeds in faeces

We used three one kilometre transects for each site in the native forests in the vicinity of the P. rufus roost. Each site was visited twice and fruit samples were taken from identified trees along the transect and stored for later determination. The seeds from these collections were used as reference samples in the identification of those extracted from the faecal samples. Faecal samples were initially assessed visually and the seeds subsequently examined with a binocular microscope (magnification: 10 x 4).

 

Flight cage experiments

We also conducted experiments at Site 1 for six nights using Plantskydd® on fruit in a tent (2m x 2m x 1.5m) into which a single R. madagascariensis was introduced and could fly freely and choose between treated and untreated fruits. For each of six nights 46 litchi fruits were provided in the cage, 23 of which had been sprayed with Plantskydd® and 23 left untreated. Three bananas fruits without Plantskydd® were also left in the cage as alternative food. The bat was left alone in the tent throughout the night and released where it was captured two hours before sunrise, and the remaining fruits with and without Plantskydd® were counted and recorded. Fruits sprayed by Plantskydd® were distinctively coloured red-brown.

 

Statistics

The three studied covariates were, the control groups, the groups impregnated with Plantskydd® and those protected with plastic flags. Linear models and ANOVA were used to investigate the effects of treatments.

 

 

314042.jpg

 

314043.jpg

 

 

 

RESULTS

 

The diet of Pteropus rufus

Faecal samples (n=277) collected from the two study sites were similar in composition so the results were pooled. They consisted of vegetable fibre (28.5%; n=66), seeds (22.0%; n=61) and consistent viscous components (19.5%: n=47). Many of the samples were green and had a pasty consistency (30.0%; n=54). The viscous matter appeared smoother and slightly transparent, but the pasty matter was compact and dense.

From these observations and the reference samples, six principal plant species were found to be eaten by P. rufus during the study period (Fig. 1). Two were introduced: Litchi chinensis (19.0%), which is commercially important and Syzygium jambos (5.0%). Three were fig species indigenous to the sites: Ficus polita, F. pyrifolia, and an unidentified Ficus sp., which made up 41.3% of the items in the faeces. Two plant species in the diet could not be identified.

 

Comparison of damage by fruit bats to litchis with different methods of deterrence: plastic flag and Plantskydd®

(1) Site 1

Bat damage to litchi fruits varied significantly according to the method of deterrence used (Table 1). The level of damage when fruit groups were protected with Plantskydd® was significantly lower than when plastic flags were used, and damage to fruits in the control groups was highest (ANOVA, F=6.093, Df=2; p<0.05). When plastic flags were used, there was a positive relationship between the number of days from the beginning of the experiment and the number of litchi fruits eaten (Pearson’s: r=0.48, t=3.25, Df=35, P=0.002) suggesting that the bats may become accustomed to the presence of the flags and ignored them after some time (Fig 2).

 

Flight cage experiments

Rousettus madagascariensis avoided feeding on litchi fruits sprayed with Plantskydd®. (Anova, F=10.537, Df=13, P<0.001). The total numbers of fruits remaining after the night’s experiments were 89 (95%) with Plantskydd® and 2 (5%) without Plantskydd®. respectively.

 

(2) Site 2

The number of bat-damaged litchi fruits per fruit group varied significantly with the method of deterrence used (Anova, F=6.093, Df=2, P≤0.01). Damage was higher in control groups than in the groups in which plastic flags and Plantskydd® were used (control groups: mean number of damaged fruits per fruit group=11.3±19.1; n samples=32), (plastic flags: mean number of damaged fruits per fruit group=0.5±1.5; n samples=20) and (Plantskydd®: mean number of damaged fruits per fruit group=2.07±1.80 n samples=27) (Fig. 3a). However, in contrast to Site 1, there was no significant correlation between the number of data collection days and the number of litchi fruits damaged by bats in the vicinity of plastic flags (Pearson’s: r=-0.1, P=0.22, Df=135) (Fig 3), meaning that regardless of the duration the flag was left with the litchi fruits, the fruit bats did not become accustomed to this deterrent and avoided feeding on the fruits.

 

Costs associated with the damage caused by fruit bats

The damage caused by bats to litchi fruits was assessed in plantations where no attempt had been made to deter bats. The maximum loss of litchi fruits to bats at Site 1 was 483 litchi fruits per day per tree. A single litchi fruit weighed from 12–18 g (average 16±4 g). The estimated average litchi productivity per tree was 4316±2101 fruits, range 1656–9425 fruits (n = 21). The fruits lost to bats amount to ca. 8kg per tree per night during the entire fruiting season. One kilogram costs approximately 700 Ariary for local purchasers, consequently the loss to the primary producer of litchi fruits due to fruit bats was around 5,600 Ariary or 2.00 USD per tree per night.

 

Predictions associated with fruit bat damage to litchis

At the beginning of our study, we predicted that two factors affected the bats’ choice of litchi tree on which to feed—the productivity of the tree and its height. We assumed that the larger the number of fruits on a tree, the more the bats would be attracted to feed there. There was no significant correlation, however, between the number of fruits eaten by the bats and tree productivity (Pearson’s: r=0.2, P=0.15, Df=180).

The second prediction was that P. rufus preferred to feed on higher trees. To investigate this, the litchi trees were categorized in three levels: low [5–8 m], medium [8–10] and high [10–14m]. There was no significant difference, however, in bat damage to fruit between these three height categories (ANOVA, Df=2, F=0.268 and P=0.76). These results suggest that neither the tree productivity nor the height of the fruits from the ground affected the bat feeding patterns.

 

Using bells to deter bats from feeding on fruit

During our bell ringing trials, we recorded 44 visits of P. rufus to our litchi target fruit groups over four consecutive nights. Two principal variables affected the effectiveness of the bell ringing method:

(1) The distance between the bell and the branch on which P. rufus fed. This observation was made intensively at Site 1. There was a positive correlation (Spearman’s correlation test r=0.85, N=23, P<0.001, df=21) between the number of bell strokes required to frighten the bats and the distance between the bell and the bats (Fig 4a). Consequently, the duration of bell ringing required to deter the bats increased proportionally with the distance between the bell and P. rufus.

(2) The location of the site: bell ringing was more effective at Site 1 than at Site 2 (Anova, F=19.149, Df=1, N=238, P<0.001). The duration of ringing required to frighten the bats away from the target fruits averaged 17±10 and 29±26 seconds in Site 1 and Site 2 respectively (Fig. 4b).

There was no significant correlation between the number of bell rings required to deter the bats and the number of the days (n=4) over which the trial was conducted (Anova, Df=38, P=0.2). Thus, over the relatively short duration of this trial the bats did not become habituated to the noise of the bell. Bell ringing was effective as around 80% of the P. rufus individuals experiencing the ringing of the bells flew away.

 

Other animal feeders

(1) Site 1: Regardless of the method of deterrence used, we found that birds fed on the studied fruit groups. Even though the average number of fruits damaged by birds in the control groups was high, there was no significant difference in the damage they caused (Anova, F=2.183, Df=2, P=0.132) between the three types of trials (fruit groups with plastic flag, with Plantskydd® and the control groups) (Fig 5 a). However, there was a significant variation in the damage to the fruit groups by unidentified feeders (especially mammals) between those three sample sets (Anova, Df=2, P=0.02). This was higher in the plastic flag experiments and control fruit groups than in fruit panicles impregnated with Plantskydd® (Fig. 5b).

(2) Site 2: The fruit groups where plastic flags were used were more heavily damaged by birds compared to those treated with Plantskydd® and the control fruit groups (Anova, df=2, P<0.05) (Fig. 6a). However, the unidentified animal feeders did not distinguish between the three samples of fruit groups (Anova, Df=2, P=0.79) (Fig. 6b).

 

Comparison between sites

Comparison of the numbers of fruit damaged in the control fruit groups revealed a significant difference between the two sites. It was higher at Site 2 (mean: 22.45±7.44, n=75), than at Site 1 (mean: 12.73±5.84, n=58) (Anova, Df=1, P<0.001) (Table 2).

No significant difference was apparent between the number of fruits damaged by bats or by other identified feeders between the two sites. In contrast, birds caused more damage at Site 2 than at Site 1. Consequently, the damage to litchi fruits could not only be attributed to bats but also to birds, with the latter causing high levels of fruit loss compared to bats at Site 2.

 

Assessing the natural damage to litchi productivity as a result of fallen fruits

Natural fruit fall (due to wind and/or rain) was assessed throughout the study period (as grounded fruit with no sign of animal damage) and was higher at Site 2 (n sampled trees=37) than at Site 1 (n sampled trees=57, ANOVA, Df=1, F=18.821, P≤0.05). The total number of natural fruit falls (apart from animals’ activity) per tree is summarized in Fig. 7.

Cost analysis of the different methods of deterring fruit bats

The costs and effectiveness of the three experimental deterrence methods are compared in Table 3.

Plantskydd® is effective for up to 16 weeks after treatment of the fruits and a single spray is sufficient to protect the fruits throughout the entire fruiting season. The litchi grower can procure this product in 1kg packages for 54.5 USD, equivalent to 137,340 Ariary, but it has to be imported from the US or Europe. The quantity required for one litchi tree is between 0.25–0.5 kg depending on the density of fruit on the tree and the price of Plantskydd® enough for one tree ranged from 27,000–34,000 Ariary or between 9.38 to 11.81 USD.

The capital cost for using bells is 108,000 Ariary per tree since a bell costs 27,000 Ariary. Farmers can store them after use and use them again in subsequent fruiting seasons and we assume they can be used for 20 years. The cost for using such a method per tree during a single fruit season is 1,664 Ariary or 0.57 USD.

Flags are made from plastic bags and are the cheapest method of deterrence (300 Ariary per flag without the installation cost). The flags tear after 21–28 days and have to be replaced. The flags need to be installed when the fruits start to ripen and attract mammals. The cost of six plastic bags was 1,800 Ariary or 0.63 USD for a single tree per fruit season.

The effective cost associated with each method was assessed from the data collected at Site 1 (Table 3). The average weight of litchi fruits was 16g, and the average litchi productivity was around 6316 fruits per tree, or about 101kg. Consequently, the financial gain during a single litchi fruit season from using the three control methods was greatest using plastic flags, followed by bell ringing and lastly Plantskydd®

314045.jpg
313515.jpg

 

 

314047.jpg

 

 

314048.jpg

 

 

313188.jpg

DISCUSSION

 

Flying fox diet

The results show that the Madagascar Flying Fox P. rufus feeds mainly on fruits and leaves with a diet similar to that of the two other Madagascar fruit bats: E. dupreanum and R. madagascariensis (Ratrimomanarivo 2003; Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2012). Only three of the seven plant species collected along the transects were present in the faecal samples collected beneath the tree roosts and three species prevalent in the faecal samples were not observed on transects: two Ficus spp and F. pyrifolia. We therefore assume that our transect samples did not represent the complete range of bat food species available in our study area. Previous studies conducted at the two sites showed that apart from L. chinensis, the bats fed on other introduced and economically important plant species such as D. longan (Sapindaceae) which seemed to be one of the most important food resources of R. madagascariensis at Site 2 (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2007) as well as banana fruit (Musaceae) which were also eaten by this species in the south-east of the island (Goodman 1999). Pteropus rufus actively consumed the pollen and nectar of Agava sisalana (Agavaceae), three species of Ficus and other introduced species not economically important (Bollen & van Elsacker 2002; Raheriarisena 2005; Long & Racey 2007; Picot et al. 2007). Those findings coupled with ours suggested that the Moraceae (particularly Ficus) and the Sapindaceae are key food resources for Madagascar’s three endemic fruit bat species.

The shift in bat feeding to introduced plant species such as litchi could be a response to habitat change. The damage to litchi fruits is assumed to be affected by their proximity to the bat roost and to the reduced availability of native fruits. Two reasons are presented to explain such findings.

Firstly, fruit bat foraging activity is influenced by food availability. Habitat destruction has resulted in the loss of native species which constituted the principal food of the bats and forced them to feed in the agricultural matrix. Rahaingodrahety et al. (2008) working also at Site 2 found that the population size of P. rufus was about 400 individuals, contrasting with our count in 2012–2013, when it had more than doubled, resulting in an increased demand for food. If native plants at the site are insufficient to meet that demand then the bats will feed on introduced plants regardless of their economic importance.

Secondly, results from other studies demonstrated that the flying foxes choose the most nutritious food. Andrianaivoarivelo et al. (2012) found that commercially important plant species were characterized by high levels of carbohydrates such as fructose compared to other forest or commercially unimportant fruit categories. The results from their feeding experiment on R. madagascariensis, however, showed that this species preferred to feed on economically unimportant forest plants which are richer in protein and lipid and this could also be the reason for the high proportion of Ficus sp. seeds we found in the P. rufus faecal samples collected during this study. For these reasons, the conservation of remaining forest and plantations of commercially unimportant plant species are recommended for mitigating the conflicts between fruit bats and litchi farmers.

 

Visual and biological bat deterrents

We found that the levels of success resulting from the use of the different means of deterrence we tested to reduce fruit bat damage were significantly different.

All three methods of deterrence investigated (visual and biological) are effective and could help the farmers to reduce fruit loss to fruit bats. Plantskydd® was the most effective especially at Site 1. The fact that the product was less effective at Site 2 was likely due to the higher rainfall there. The frequent and unpredictable rain appears to have washed the Plantskydd® from some of our target fruit groups and reduced its effectiveness especially when it was applied less than six hours before the rain. At Site 2, rain fell unpredictably, any time of the day and sometimes soon after we had sprayed the fruits with Plantskydd® , so we had to repeat the spraying.

Plastic flags were less effective at both sites than Plantskydd® but was the most commonly used method at Site 1, although after they have been used for several days, the bats become habituated to them. Also, P. rufus has well developed vision for some colours (Müller et al. 2007) and the plastic flags may act as a guide directing them to the litchi plantation and fruits. The data collected at Site 2 however demonstrated that the number of litchi fruits eaten by the fruit bats decreased when plastic flags were used. Here no deterrent had previously been used by the farmers apart from killing the fruit bats with sticks or shooting them (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. 2007) and hunting at the roost. At Site 2, plastic flags were almost as effective as Plantskydd®. The latter was much more effective at Site 1.

 

Bell ringing

Each of our bell ringing experiments lasted for four days but we found no evidence of bats habituating to the bell strokes and feeding on fruits near the bell and there was no significant correlation between the duration of the experiment and the duration of bell ringing. Bell ringing disturbed the bats and made them fly away from the fruits. The extent to which bells remain effective beyond four days should be investigated. Their effectiveness depended on the distance between the bell and the feeding bats and perhaps by the level of bat hunger or starvation.

In Australia, sound was initially effective but not recommended for long term use because flying foxes became habituated (Bicknell 2002). For this reason, we expect that after four or more days of rings, the number of rings needed to frighten away the flying foxes would increase and P. rufus would become accustomed to the sounds and continue feeding on the litchi fruits.

Overall, the fruit protection methods employed by the litchis growers were not reliably effective except for the biological method we tested—Plantskydd®. The majority of the households in a village in Kenya used a combination of two or more protection methods simultaneously such as guarding, scarecrows, beating on objects to make noises and also by harvesting immature crops (Musyoki 2014). None of the methods alone were 100% effective so the combined effect of such methods helped to increase their success for better controlling fruit loss due to the bats.

We also found that the level of success of each method varied between the sites. Several factors may interplay to explain this such as the climate, food availability and size of the bat population. The effectiveness of Plantskydd® at Site 2 was limited by the rain that washed the product off the fruit.

 

Other fruit pests

Fruit bats were not the only animal group feeding on litchi plantations. A large proportion of the damage to litchis was attributed to omnivorous and granivorous birds such as Coracopsis spp and the introduced Acridotheres tristis which were seen eating the litchis. We observed at Site 2 that C. vaza, C. nigra and A. tristis destroyed fruit which they ate only partially before they began to peel and tear apart another fruit. Such devastating bird activity was more prevalent at Site 2 than at Site 1. The birds fed on fruits treated with Plantskydd® so they were not deterred by its odour and taste.

According to Marsh et al. (1992), the effectiveness of plastic flags in deterring birds from feeding in litchi orchards is enhanced by loud distress sounds. Using only one method could ensure fruit protection but only for a limited time because the birds rapidly become habituated to the flags (Marsh et al. 1992). Birds feed actively on litchi fruits even though plastic flags are installed in the trees.

We also observed that fruits were missing as a result of the activity of unidentified feeders. This could partially be due to R. madagascariensis. During our nocturnal observation (data not collected), R. madagascariensis individuals were occasionally observed eating the fruits but their feeding behavior was quite different to P. rufus. They hovered above the fruit panicle then quickly removed one fruit with their mouth and carried it away. Such behaviour was probably adapted to reduce the risk of predation or to minimize competition with other animals feeding on the same fruit panicle. No lemurs were seen during our investigation, and this may be due to the long distance separating the litchi trees and known lemur habitats.

Fruit predation by bats elsewhere

In Mauritius, litchis are grown in two situations, orchards and backyards. New orchards are pruned low and panicles are netted or nets thrown over the whole trees to protect the fruits from bats and birds. Backyard trees are too tall for netting and besides, the fruit of the tree is often sold each season to a merchant who owns it until it is picked and has little incentive to buy nets. In Thailand and Queensland Australia, the only method of crop protection that is really effective in preventing birds and mammals feeding on orchards is the use of physical barriers, such as full netting (Rigden et al. 2000; Aziz et al. 2016). This method is difficult to install in litchi plantations in Madagascar as it is costly and the litchis trees are not grouped to form an orchard and are not pruned, so that they reach heights of up to 20m. In conclusion, the use of aversive taste deterrents is the most effective method of reducing fruit loss to bats. In Madagascar, however, litchi farmers struggle with two main factors: they have no access to Plantskydd® and their choice of deterrent will be largely dictated by cost. Nevertheless, Plantskydd® should be tested in other countries and on other species of commercial fruit where full netting is impractical or unaffordable.

References

 

Aziz, S.A., K.J. Olival, S. Bumrungsri, G.C. Richards & P.A. Racey (2016). The conflict between fruit bats and fruit growers: species, legislation and mitigation, Pp. 377–426. In: Voigt, C.C. & T. Kingston (eds). Bats in the Anthropocene - Conservation of Bats in a Changing World. Springer- New York, 606pp.

Andrianaivoarivelo, A.R., O.R. Ramilijaona & D. Andriafidison (2007). Rousettus madagascariensis Grandidier 1929 feeding on Dimnocarpus longan in Madagascar. African Bat Conservation News 11: 3–4.

Andrianaivoarivelo, A.R., R.K.B. Jenkins, E.J. Petit, O. Ramilijaona, N. Razafindrakoto & P.A. Racey (2012). Rousettus madagascariensis (Chiroptera: Pteropodidae) shows a preference for native and commercially unimportant fruits. Endangered Species Research 19: 19–27.

Bicknell, J. (2002). The need for aversion agents for managing flying-foxes on crops and the difficulties in attracting research funds. pp. 63–69. In: Eby, P. & D. Lunney (eds.). Managing the Grey-headed Flying-fox as a Threatened Species in NSW. Royal Zoological Society of NSW, Mosman, 284pp.

Bollen, A. & L. van Elsacker (2002). Feeding ecology of Pteropus rufus (Pteropodidae) in the littoral forest of Sainte Luce, SE Madagascar. Acta Chiropterologica 4: 33–47.

Goodman, S.M. (1999). Notes on the bats of the Réserve Intégrale d’Andohahela and surrounding areas of southeastern Madagascar. Fieldiana: Zoology, new series 94: 251–257.

Long, E. & P.A. Racey (2007). An exotic plantation crop as a keystone resource for an endemic megachiropteran, Pteropus rufus, in Madagascar. Journal of Tropical Ecology 23: 397–407.

Marsh, R.E., W.A. Erickson & T.P. Salmon (1992). Scarecrows and predator models for frightening birds from specific area. Proceedings of the Fifteenth Vertebrate Pest Conference. Paper 49: 111–114.

Menzel, C. (2002). The Litchis Crop in Asia and the Pacific. Maroochy Research Station, Queensland Department of Primary Industries, scmc, Nambour, Australia, Rap Publication 16: 102pp.

Müller, B., S.M. Goodman & L. Peichl (2007). Cone photoreceptor diversity in the retinas of fruit bats (Megachiroptera). Brain Behavior and Evolution 70: 90–104.

Musyoki, C. (2014). Crop defense and coping strategies: Wildlife raids in Mahiga ‘B’ village in Nyeri District, Kenya. African Study Monographs 35(1): 19–40.

Picot, M., R.K.B. Jenkins, O.R. Ramilijaona, P.A. Racey & S.M. Carrière (2007). The feeding ecology of Eidolon dupreanum (Pteropodidae) in eastern Madagascar. African Journal of Ecology 45: 645–650.

Rahaingodrahety, V.N., D. Andriafidison, J.H. Ratsimbazafy, P.A. Racey & R.K.B. Jenkins (2008). Three Flying Fox (Pteropodidae: Pteropus rufus) roosts, three conservation challenges in southeastern Madagascar. Madagascar Conservation & Development 3(1): 17–21.

Raheriarisena, M. (2005). Régime alimentaire de Pteropus rufus (Chiroptera: Pteropodidae) dans la région subaride du sud de Madagascar. Revue d‘Ecologie (Terre et Vie) 60: 255–264.

Ratrimomanarivo, H.F. (2003). Etude du régime alimentaire d’un Mégachiroptère Eidolon dupreanum, Pollen 1866 dans les régions des Hautes-Terres Malgaches et son rôle dans les régénérations des plantes dans l’écosystème modifié par l’homme. Mémoire de D.E.A., Université d’Antananarivo, Madagascar.

Rigden, P., J. Page & J. Chapman (2000). To Net or Not to Net? Flying-Fox Control in Orchards through Netting Protection. Queensland Horticultural Institute, Queensland Department of Primary Industries, Nambour, Queensland, 64pp.

Stashko, E.R., & E. Dinerstein (1988). Methods of estimating fruit availability to frugivorous bats, pp. 221–231. In: Kunz, T.H. (ed.). Ecological and Behavioral Methods for the Study of Bats. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC.

Sud, U.C., T. Ahmad, V.K. Gupta, H. Chandra, P.M. Sahoo, K. Aditya, M. Singh & A. Biswas (2015). Research on Improving Methods for Estimating Crop Area, Yield and Production under Mixed, Repeated and Continuous Cropping ICAR-Indian Agricultural Statistics Research Institute New Delhi, India, 119pp.

Tang, Z., L. Sheng, X. Ma, M. Cao, S. Parsons, J. Ma & S. Zang (2007). Temporal and spatial patterns of seed dispersal of Musa acuminata by Cynopterus sphinx. Acta Chiropterologica 9(1): 229–235.

Wagner, K.K. & D.L. Nolte (2001). Comparison of active ingredients and delivery systems in deer repellents. Wildlife Society Bulletin 29(1): 322–333.