Article

Journal of Threatened Taxa | www.threatenedtaxa.org | 26 September 2017 | 9(9): 10642–10648

 

275477.jpg

 

 

 

 

Effects of the invasive Tilapia on the Common Spiny Loach (Cypriniformes: Cobitidae: Lepidocephalichthys thermalis) - implications for conservation

Sandip D. Tapkir 1, Sanjay S. Kharat 2, Pradeep Kumkar 3 & Sachin M. Gosavi 4

1,2,3,4 Department of Zoology, Modern College of Arts, Science and Commerce, Ganeshkhind Road, Opposite University Gate, Ganeshkhind, Pune, Maharashtra 411016, India

1 Department of Zoology, Savitribai Phule Pune University, Ganeshkhind, Pune, Maharashtra 411007, India

4 Department of Zoology, Modern College of Arts, Science and Commerce, Shivajinagar, Pune, Maharashtra 411005, India

4 Department of Zoology, Maharashtra College of Arts, Science and Commerce, 246-A, Jahangir Boman Behram Marg, Bellasis Road, Mumbai, Maharashtra 400008 India

1 sandiptapkir1@gmail.com, 2 kharat.sanjay@gmail.com, 3 kumkarpradeep@gmail.com, 4 schn.gosavi@gmail.com (corresponding author)

 

 

 

 

 

doi: http://doi.org/10.11609/jott.2220.9.9.10642-10648 | ZooBank: urn:lsid:zoobank.org:pub:B321059E-07F1-4E24-B8B3-0E066C297C01

 

Editor: Davor Zanella, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia. Date of publication: 26 September 2017 (online & print)

 

Manuscript details: Ms # 2220 | Received 24 August 2015 | Final received 05 August 2017 | Finally accepted 17 August 2017

 

Citation: Tapkir, S.D., S.S. Kharat, P. Kumkar & S.M. Gosavi (2017). Effects of the invasive Tilapia on the Common Spiny Loach (Cypriniformes: Cobitidae: Lepidocephalichthys thermalis) - implications for conservation. Journal of Threatened Taxa 9(9): 10642–10648; http://doi.org/10.11609/jott.2220.9.9.10642-10648

 

Copyright: © Tapkir et al 2017. Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. JoTT allows unrestricted use of this article in any medium, reproduction and distribution by providing adequate credit to the authors and the source of publication.

 

Funding: University Grant Commission (UGC) file number 47-914/14 (WRO) New Delhi.

 

Competing interests: The authors declare no competing interests.

 

Author Details: Sandip D. Tapkir is working on the prey predator interactions in aquatic organisms, gut microbiome of the animals and also interested in freshwater fish diversity. Sanjay S. Kharat is working on diversity and distribution of freshwater fishes in the northern Western Ghats of India. Pradeep Kumkar is working on diversity, distribution, ecology and evolution of freshwater fishes. Sachin M. Gosavi is a PhD student, conducting his doctoral study in Department of Zoology, Modern College, Ganeshkhind. He works on behavior biology and ecology of aquatic animals.

 

Author Contribution: SDT and SMG designed the experiments with the help of SSK. SDT, SMG and PK performed experiments and wrote the manuscript with inputs from SSK.

 

Acknowledgements: We thank anonymous reviewers and subject editors for constructive comments on the earlier version of manuscript. The research was supported by grants from University Grant Commission (UGC) under minor research project, file number 47-914/14 (WRO) New Delhi sanctioned to Sanjay Kharat. We wish to acknowledge Neelesh Dahanukar, Indian Institute of Science Education and Research (IISER), Pune for his constant support, encouragement and help in data analysis.

 

 

 

 

Abstract: The introduction of invasive species leading to decline of freshwater fauna is a major concern for conservation biologists. In this study we examined the effect of introduced Tilapia on the survival of the loach Lepidocephalichthys thermalis via predation experiments with Tilapia and a native predator, the Snakehead fish Channa gachua. Examining the chemoecology of prey-predator interactions, we found that L. thermalis failed to detect water-borne cues from Tilapia but did recognize cues from C. gachua, indicating innate predator recognition. We also observed that L. thermalis can learn to associate kairomones with Tilapia when conditioned with kairomones and injured conspecific cues. Trained L. thermalis showed higher survival during Tilapia predation trials. Thus under experimental conditions the vulnerability of L. thermalis to Tilapia predation due to failure to detect chemical cues can be reduced via associative training. It remains to be determined how useful this behavioral plasticity can be in wild L. thermalis populations exposed to introduced Tilapia.

 

 

Keywords: Behavior, chemical cues, invasion, kairomones, Oreochromis mossambicus, predator recognition.

 

 

 

 

INTRODUCTION

More than 2,000 freshwater fish species are known to occur in India (Devi 2003; Dahanukar et al. 2004; Raghavan et al. 2013), with many new species described from the Western Ghats (Dahanukar et al. 2015), a global biodiversity hotspot (Myers et al. 2000). Unfortunately, endemic species are declining at high rates, which has been attributed to several factors including the introduction of invasive species (Molur et al. 2011). In the Western Ghats 324 invasive fish species have been identified, and this invasion has been linked to the loss of native populations (Johnson et al. 2014; Keskar et al. 2014). The Tilapia Oreochromis mossambicus (Peters, 1852), is one of the most harmful invasive species in aquatic ecosystems worldwide (Lowe et al. 2004) and it was introduced to India for aquaculture in 1952 (de silva et al. 2004). Tilapia now occupies wide variety of aquatic habitats (e.g., estuarine and free-flowing, lakes, watercourses and wetlands) shared with a range of endemic freshwater fish, including loaches. The Western Ghats harbors 43 loach species, of which 39 are endemic (Dahanukar & Raghavan 2013). Loach populations are declining in several areas, where a major factor is the introduction of invasive species (Paingankar & Dahanukar 2013; Keskar et al. 2014), including Tilapia. The chemoecology of prey-predator interactions in fish has been extensively investigated (Ferrari et al. 2010). While prey species often possess innate abilities to recognize native predators via water-borne chemical cues (Ferrari et al. 2010), they often fail to recognize predators with which they do not share an evolutionary history (Polo-Cavia et al. 2010; Gomez-Mestre & Diaz-Paniagua 2011). Thus invasive predators can exploit novel ecological contexts where prey populations lack sufficient anti-predator adaptations to limit predation pressure (Shea & Chesson 2002). It is, however, sometimes possible for prey to learn to recognize invasive predators under experimental conditions (Ferrari et al. 2010), suggesting that given sufficient opportunities they may be able to adapt to invaders in the wild.

In this study we performed chemoecology experiments using the Common Spiny Loach Lepidocephalichthys thermalis as prey, and the Snakehead fish Channa gachua and Tilapia as a native and invasive predator respectively. Our aim was to determine: (1) Does L. thermalis innately recognize the native and/or invasive predator species? (2) In the absence of an innate response, can L. thermalis learn to recognize predators? (3) Can such learning enhance the survival of L. thermalis in predator encounters?

 

 

 

 

MATERIALS AND METHODS

 

Animal collection and maintenance

Live L. thermalis (n = 63; hereafter test fish) were collected from a local fish market at Lonawala (18.747 N & 73.449 E), Maharashtra (Fig. 1). Fish were transported in plastic bags filled with dechlorinated oxygenated water to the laboratory, where they were maintained in large aerator-equipped aquariums (120 x 60 x 60 cm) filled with dechlorinated tap water (20 L) at 250C under a natural photoperiod (12L : 12D). Live specimens of Snakehead (n = 2) and Tilapia (n = 20) were acquired from the same fish market and housed in separate aquaria under the same conditions. Water was renewed thrice weekly and fish were fed twice daily with artificial fish food/blood worms. All animals were allowed to acclimate for 20 days prior to experimentation.

 

Preparation of chemical stimuli

According to Peacor et al. (2006), water-borne cues last for 2–4 days. Predator chemical cues were prepared by filling aquaria with dechlorinated tap water and introducing either Snakehead or Tilapia; after 48hr of occupancy the fish were removed and the conditioned water was frozen in 50mL aliquots (Gomez-Mestre & Diaz-Paniagua 2011). Conditioned water was collected both from starved fish, which were not fed for two days prior to transfer (to clean their gut) nor during occupancy, and from satiated fish that were fed with L. thermalis once a day for two days prior to and during occupancy. A negative control was prepared using distilled water following the same procedure without exposure to fish (Woody & Mathis 1998).

Prey alarm cues were collected from L. thermalis donors (n=3) that were euthanized using highly concentrated MS-222 (Tricane methane sulphonate) solution, washed thoroughly in distilled water and snap-frozen in liquid nitrogen. Skin pieces (1cm2) were removed from both sides of the body and placed in 10ml chilled distilled water prior to homogenization and straining (Manek et al. 2013). This solution was diluted to 10L and frozen in 50ml aliquots (Woody & Mathis 1998).

 

Prey response to predator cues

The response of L. thermalis to water-borne chemical cues was measured using the well-established protocol of Batabyal et al. (2014). Briefly, individual fish were introduced to a testing chamber (100 × 12 × 6 cm) with 10 equal sections of 10cm length denoted by line markings. The chamber was cleaned and filled with 2L of dechlorinated tap water prior to introduction of a test fish, which was allowed to acclimate for 20 min. Pre-stimulus activity was measured by counting the number of lines crossed by the test fish during a period of four minutes. 5ml aliquots of test solution were then added to each end of the testing chamber, and any chemical cues present were allowed to diffuse for five minutes (determined via a dye test using KMnO4 per Batabyal et al. 2014). Post-stimulus activity was then recorded for four minutes. Stimulus response was calculated by subtracting pre-stimulus from post-stimulus readings; a negative value indicated reduced activity, a positive value increased activity. In total, 20 individual fish across five different treatments were used. Observers were blinded as to the test solution used in each trial.

 

Prey conditioning

L. thermalis individuals (n= 40) were randomly assigned to two groups in separate aquaria. Each aquarium received 10ml of starved Tilapia-conditioned solution, and one aquarium (“conditioned”) also received 10ml of L. thermalis alarm cue solution. After 24h, fish from both aquaria were tested for response to Tilapia predator cues or distilled water as described above.

 

Prey survival

Fish from conditioned and non-conditioned aquaria (above) were randomly sorted into pairs (n = 20 pairs), whose members identified by distinctive morphological features. Pair members were simultaneously introduced to separate aquaria filled with 20L of dechlorinated tap water containing one Tilapia that had been starved for 48h. The time was recorded until one fish was eaten or killed by the Tilapia, and the identity of the survivor was recorded.

 

Data analysis

Shapiro-Wilks test for normality revealed non-normal distribution of data. Therefore, to compare the swimming activity of test fish in presence of different chemical cues for both Expt. 1 and Expt. 2, we used a Kruskal–Wallis H test followed by Mann-Whitney U-test for multiple pairwise comparison with Bonferroni correction. In Expt. 3, survival in predation trials was analyzed using Fisher’s exact test. All results were considered significant at α = 0.05. The statistical analysis was carried out using PAST 3.13 (Hammer et al. 2001).

 

316140.jpg

 

 

 

RESULTS

 

Prey response to predator cues

A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed that there was a statistically significant difference in swimming activity of test fish between predator cue treatments (χ2 = 31.36, P < 0.0001). Post-hoc tests revealed that test fish reduced their swimming activity significantly in the presence of starved Snakehead cues, compared to their activity in distilled water (Mann-Whitney U-test = 50.5; P = 0.0005; Fig. 2). Test fish reduced activity even further when exposed to cues from satiated Snakehead that had fed on conspecific prey (U = 48; P = 0.0004; Fig. 2). The test fish, however, did not reduce their activity level in the presence of chemical cues from invasive Tilapia, regardless of whether Tilapia were satiated (U = 184; P = 1; Fig. 2) or starved (U = 182; P = 1; Fig. 2).

 

Prey conditioning

A statistically significant difference was observed in the swimming activity of test fish (χ2 = 15.52, P = 0.0014; Fig. 3) between conditioned and non-conditioned treatment. Multiple pair-wise comparisons showed that, test fish conditioned with conspecific alarm cues added in combination with Tilapia cues significantly reduced swimming activity in the presence of Tilapia cues compared to distilled water (U= 66.5; P = 0.0019; Fig. 3), whereas activity of non-conditioned test fish did not vary in the presence of Tilapia cues compared to distilled water (U = 189.5; P = 1; Fig. 3). In addition, swimming activity of conditioned and non-conditioned test fish did not differ significantly in distilled water (U = 160; P = 1; Fig. 3), but conditioned test fish were comparatively less active than non-conditioned test fish in presence of Tilapia cues (U = 104; P = 0.05; Fig. 3).

 

Prey survival

In real predation trials using starved Tilapia, conditioned test fish had significantly higher overall survival than non-conditioned test fish (Fisher exact test, P = 0.0038; Fig. 4). Non-conditioned test fish were often consumed before conditioned test fish due to their higher swimming activity. In total, 20 out of 40 test fish were eaten, 15 (75%) of them non-conditioned and five (25%) conditioned.

 

 

316142.jpg

 

316143.jpg

DISCUSSION

 

Lepidocephalichthys thermalis possess innate predator recognition towards the Snakehead (native predator), since they detect water- borne chemical cues from starved Snakehead without prior experience and adaptively reduce activity. Several other aquatic species (freshwater snails, salmonoid fishes, larval toads and salamanders) are also known to display innate predator recognition wherein prior exposure to the predator is not essential (Ferrari et al. 2010). Furthermore, the anti-predator responses were much stronger when L. thermalis were tested against the chemical cue from satiated Snakehead, indicating a synergistic effect of Snakehead kairomones and dietary cues. In contrast, L. thermalis failed to discriminate chemical cues from invasive Tilapia, regardless of whether Tilapia was starved or satiated. There are two plausible reasons behind such lack of responses. Firstly, development of innate predator recognition requires a long evolutionary history between the prey and predator (Ecological coexistence theory; Kiesecker & Blaustein 1997). Invasive Tilapia was introduced in India in 1952 (de silva et al. 2004), however, according to documentary evidences Tilapia was recorded from the study area for the first time in 2012 (Dahanukar et al. 2012). Previous investigations on fish fauna of Indrayani River by Sykes (1839), Suter (1944) and Yazdani & Mahabal (1976) showed no records for Tilapia occurrence in study area. This indicates that Tilapia was introduced in the study area after 1976. For instance, even if we consider that Tilapia was introduced in the study area in 1976, it is just 39 years after introduction of Tilapia, which may not be sufficient evolutionary time span to develop innate predator recognition in L. thermalis. This suggests that the lack of innate recognition of invasive Tilapia likely to be common in other loach species such as Acanthocobitis mooreh, Oreonectes evezardi, Nemachilichthys rueppelli, Noemacheilus anguilla, Schistura denisoni inhabiting the study area. Similar study performed by Polo-Cavia & Gomez-Mestre (2014) on Western Spadefoot Toad Pelobates cultripes showed that tadpoles of the spadefoot toads fail to detect the chemical cues innately from the invasive Red Swamp Crayfish Procambarus clarkii even after 35 years of its introduction. Yet, some amphibian species are known to adapt to novel thermal regimes via behavioral plasticity within a 30-year time frame (Skelly & Freidenburg 2000; Freidenburg & Skelly 2004). Second plausible reason may be higher generation times of L. thermalis, but currently no data is available on its reproductive biology. Due to the higher generation time responses of L. thermalis to selection for innate recognition of introduced predators should be slow. We suggest that, detailed investigation of the reproductive biology of L. thermalis is needed to understand the role of higher generation time in shaping the lack of anti-predator responses against Tilapia.

Post-digestion or dietary cues are also known to elicit anti-predatory responses against the novel predators in aquatic prey (Ferrari et al. 2010; Nunes et al. 2013), but in case of the L. thermalis presence of even dietary cues may not be sufficient to trigger recognition of invasive predator. This could be explained in terms of the phylogenetic-relatedness theory. The ‘Phylogenetic-relatedness theory’ proposes that response of prey to alarm cues or dietary cues of closely related predator species should be stronger than such cues from distantly related ones (Sullivan et al. 2003; Schoeppner & Relyea 2005). Moreover, the chemical cues (Kairomones/dietary cues) released by related predator are more similar and identified by prey species (Ferrari et al. 2007). Phylogenetically, Snakehead and Tilapia are not closely related since both belong to two different families (Channidae and Cichlidae respectively). Therefore, despite of ecological coexistence of L. thermalis with Snakehead and Tilapia, L. thermalis only detect Channa dietary cues and fails to detect kairomones as well as dietary cues from Tilapia.

The ability to learn to detect novel predators is always advantageous over an innate mechanism of predator detection since innate predator recognition has limitation for identification of number of predators (Batabyal et al. 2014). Therefore, learned predator recognition is known to be helpful in reducing the predation by invasive predators (Polo-Cavia & Gomez-Mestre 2014). In the current study, we showed that L. thermalis can learn to recognize chemical cues from invasive Tilapia as a threat by associative learning, and modify their anti-predator responses adaptively based on their experience. Such associative learning through pairing of kairomones with alarm cues has been reported in different species (Ferrari et al. 2010). It includes, flatworms, mollusks, insects, crustaceans, fishes and amphibians (as reviewed in Ferrari et al. 2010). Most of these studies, however, have been conducted in the laboratory; learning through alarm cues is likely to happen in the wild too, since prey animals are constantly exposed to a combination of predator’s kairomones and alarm cues released by attacked conspecifics (Ferrari et al. 2010). By cognitive association of these cues, prey animals adjust quickly to the surrounding environment and may dynamically adjust risk assessment, enhancing their ability to detect and avoid predators by responding plastically to new threats through changes in morphology and/or behavior. Furthermore, associative learning may provide substantial time for evolution of innate recognition of invasive predators in prey individuals.

Prior experience with predators is known to affect the prey fitness and survival. Unfortunately, most studies are only restricted to predator detection and learning. Very few studies till date have demonstrated adaptive value of learned predator recognition in direct encounters with predators (Mirza & Chivers 2000; Gazdewich & Chivers 2002; Lonnstedt et al. 2012; Polo-Cavia & Gomez-Mestre 2014). To our knowledge, this is the first study which provides empirical evidence of learned predator recognition in loaches and its effect on survival rates during direct encounters with an exotic, globally introduced predator, Tilapia. We found that, L. thermalis that learned to recognize the Tilapia by associative learning showed higher survival as compared to those that are not conditioned. Enhanced survival is likely to occur also in natural scenario, since experienced prey fishes might rapidly identify predatory Tilapia as threatening and exhibit avoidance behavior (Polo-Cavia & Gomez-Mestre 2014). Such ability to learn about the invasive predator might be important in case of the loach populations to persist in presence of invasive predators. Associative learning ability in loach populations helps to cope up with the immediate impact of invasions through behavioral plasticity and buy time for genetic variants to appear and respond to selection (Polo-Cavia & Gomez-Mestre 2014). Moreover, prey animals are also known to undergo certain changes in morphology induced by novel predators (Ferrari et al. 2010; Gomez-Mestre & Díaz-Paniagua 2011). These morphological changes allow experienced prey individuals to effectively cope with the new selective challenge without necessarily or immediately leading to genetic shifts in populations (Huey et al. 2003; Gomez-Mestre & Díaz-Paniagua 2011). But in comparison with morphological responses, behavioral responses are quick and energetically less costly due to the unpredictable nature of predation risk. Therefore, learning is the best and rapid way towards adaptation to a novel invasive predator, whereas innate responses might evolve after substantial ecological coexistence with invasive predator (West-Eberhard 2003). Hence, evolution of behavioral plasticity in terms of activity reduction seems to be beneficial for native prey animals to adapt to selective impacts from introduced predators (Strauss et al. 2006).

In conclusion, our results show that despite lacking innate recognition of invasive Tilapia, L. thermalis can learn to recognize this invasive predator by associative learning when jointly receive Tilapia cues and alarm cues from injured conspecifics. This cognitive ability of L. thermalis increases their chance of survival in direct encounters with Tilapia, which might be important for native loach populations to endure predator invasions until they develop innate predator recognition against Tilapia. We suggest that, more work with consideration of the other loach species is needed to document and better understand the effect of invasive species on loach population. This will help in determining that which loach species is most likely to be the more vulnerable for extinction due to invasion and based on this knowledge, conservation strategies can be planned.

 

 

REFERENCES

 

 

Batabyal, A., S.M. Gosavi & N.P. Gramapurohit (2014). Determining sensitive stages for learning to detect predators in larval bronzed frogs: Importance of alarm cues in learning. Journal of Biosciences 39(4): 701–710; http://doi.org/10.1007/s12038-014-9455-7

Dahanukar, N. & R. Raghavan (2013). Freshwater fishes of Western Ghats: Checklist v 1.0 August 2013. MIN - Newsletter of IUCN SSC/WI Freshwater Fish Specialist Group - South Asia and the Freshwater Fish Conservation Network of South Asia (FFCNSA) 1: 6–16.

Dahanukar, N., M. Paingankar, R.N. Raut & S. S. Kharat (2012). Fish fauna of Indrayani River, northern Western Ghats, India. Journal of Threatened Taxa 4(1): 2310–2317; http://doi.org/10.11609/JoTT.o2771.2310-7

Dahanukar, N., P. Kumkar, U. Katwate & R. Raghavan (2015). Badis britzi, a new percomorph fish (Teleostei: Badidae) from the Western Ghats of India. Zootaxa 3941(3): 429–436; http://doi.org/10.11646/zootaxa.3941.3.9

Dahanukar, N., R. Raut & A. Bhat (2004). Distribution, endemism and threat status of freshwater fishes in the Western Ghats of India. Journal of Biogeography 31(1): 123-136; http://doi.org/10.1046/j.0305-0270.2003.01016.x

de Silva, S.S., R.P. Subasinghe, D.M. Bartley & A. Lowther (2004). Tilapias as alien aquatics in Asia and the Pacific: a review. Fisheries and Aquaculture Department Fisheries Technical Paper. No. 453. Rome, FAO. 2004, 65pp.

Devi, K.R. (2003). Freshwater fish diversity. In: Venkataraman K (ed.) Natural Aquatic Ecosystems of India, Thematic Biodiversity Strategy and Action Plan. Zoological Survey of India, Kolkatta.

Ferrari, M.C.O., A. Gonzalo, F. Messier & D.P. Chivers (2007). Generalization of learned predator recognition: an experimental test and framework for future studiesProceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 274(1620): 1853–1859; http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.0297

Ferrari, M.C.O., B.D. Wisenden & D.P. Chivers (2010). Chemical ecology of predator-prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 88(7): 698–724; http://doi.org/10.1139/Z10-029

Freidenburg, L.K. & D.K. Skelly (2004). Microgeographic variation in thermal preference by an amphibian. Ecology Letters 7(5): 369–373. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00587.x

Gazdewich, K.J. & D.P. Chivers (2002). Acquired predator recognition by fathead minnows: the influence of habitat characteristics on survival. Journal of Chemical Ecology 28(2): 439–445.

Gomez-Mestre, I. & C. Diaz-Paniagua (2011). Invasive predatory crayfish do not trigger inducible defences in tadpoles. Proceeding of the Royal Society B278: 3364–3370; http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2762

Hammer, Ø., D.A.T. Harper & P.D. Ryan (2001). PAST: Paleontological statistics software package for education and data analysis. Palaeontologia Electronica 4: 9pp; http://palaeo-electronica.org/2001_1/past/issue1_01.htm

Huey, R.B., P.E. Hertz & B. Sinervo (2003). Behavioral drive versus behavioral inertia in evolution: a null model approach. American Naturalist 161(3): 357–366.

Johnson, J.A., R. Paromita & K. Sivakumar (2014). Report of new invasive fish, Red-bellied Piranha Pygocentrus nattereri in Godavari River, Andhra Pradesh. MIN - Newsletter of IUCN SSC/WI Freshwater Fish Specialist Group - South Asia and the Freshwater Fish Conservation Network of South Asia (FFCNSA) 2: 8–10.

Keskar, A., A. Padhye & N. Dahanukar (2014). Fighting against all odds: the struggle for existence among hill stream loaches of northern Western Ghats. MIN - Newsletter of IUCN SSC/WI Freshwater Fish Specialist Group - South Asia and the Freshwater Fish Conservation Network of South Asia (FFCNSA) 2: 25–29pp.

Kiesecker, J.M. & A.R. Blaustein (1997).Population differences in responses of Red-legged Frogs (Rana aurora) to introduced bullfrogs. Ecology 78(6): 1752–1760; http://doi.org/10.1890/0012-9658(1997)078[1752:PDIROR]2.0.CO;2

Lonnstedt, O., M.I. McCormick, M.G. Meekan, M.C.O. Ferrari & D.P. Chivers (2012). Learn and live: predator experience and feeding history determine prey behaviours and survival. Proceeding of the Royal Society B 279(1736): 2091–2098; http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.2516

Lowe, S., M. Browne, S. Boudjelas & M. de Poorter (2004). 100 of the World’s Worst Invasive Alien Species A selection from the Global Invasive Species Database. Published by The Invasive Species Specialist Group (ISSG) a specialist group of the Species Survival Commission (SSC) of the World Conservation Union (IUCN), 12pp.

Manek, A.K., M.C.O. Ferrari, R.J. Pollock, D. Vicente, L.P. Weber & D.P. Chivers (2013). Within and between population variation in epidermal club cell investment in a freshwater prey fish: a cautionary tale for evolutionary ecologists. PLosONE 8(3): e56689; http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056689

Mirza, R.S. & D.P. Chivers (2000). Predator-recognition training enhances survival of brook trout: evidence from laboratory and field-enclosure studies. Canadian Journal of Zoology 78(12): 2198–2208; http://doi.org/10.1139/Z10-029

Molur, S., K.G. Smith, B.A. Daniel & W.R.T. Darwall (2011). The Status and Distribution of Freshwater Biodiversity in the Western Ghats, India. Cambridge, UK and Gland, Switzerland: IUCN, and Zoo Outreach Organisation, Coimbatore, India, viii+116pp.

Myers, N., R.A. Mittermeier, C.G. Mittermeier, G.A.B. De Fonseca & J. Kent (2000). Biodiversity and hotspots for conservation priorities. Nature 403: 853–858; http://doi.org/10.1038/35002501

Nunes, A.L., A. Richter-Boix, A.Laurila & R. Rebelo (2013). Do anuran larvae respond behaviourally to chemical cues from an invasive Crayfish predator? A community-wide study. Oecologia 171(1): 115–127; http://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-012-2389-6

Paingankar, M. & N. Dahanukar (2013). Fishy aliens: invasive introduced fishes on the forts of the northern Western Ghats. MIN - Newsletter of IUCN SSC/WI Freshwater Fish Specialist Group - South Asia and the Freshwater Fish Conservation Network of South Asia (FFCNSA) 1: 33–37.

Peacor, S.D., S. Allesina, R.L. Riolo, & M. Pascual (2006). Phenotypic plasticity opposes species invasions by altering fitness surface. PLoS Biology 4: e372; http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040372

Peters, W.C.H. (1852). Diagnosen von neuen Flussfischenaus Mossambique. Monatsberichte der Königlichen Preussische Akademie des Wissenschaften zu Berlin 1852: 681–685.

Polo-Cavia, N., A. Gonzalo, P. Lopez & J. Martin (2010). Predator recognition of native but not invasive turtle predators by naïve anuran tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 80(3): 461–466; http://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12175

Polo-Cavia, N. & I. Gomez-Mestre (2014). Learned recognition of introduced predators determines survival of tadpole prey. Functional Ecology 28(2): 432-439; http://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12175

Raghavan, R., N. Dahanukar, M. Tlusty, A. Rhyne, K.K. Kumar, S. Molur & A.M. Rosser (2013). Uncovering an obscure trade: threatened freshwater fishes and the aquarium pet markets. Biological Conservation 164: 158–169; http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2013.04.019

Schoeppner, N.M. & R.A. Relyea (2005). Damage, digestion and defence: the role of alarm cues and kairomones for inducing prey defences. Ecology Letters 8(5): 505–512; http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00744.x

Shea, K. & P. Chesson (2002). Community ecology theory as a framework for biological invasions. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 17(4): 170–176; http://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02495-3

Skelly, D.K. & L.K. Freidenburg (2000). Effects of beaver on the thermal biology of an amphibian. Ecology Letters 3(6): 483–486; http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00587.x

Strauss, S.Y., J.A. Lau & S.P. Carroll (2006). Evolutionary responses of natives to introduced species: what do introductions tell us about natural communities? Ecology Letters 9(3): 357–374; http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00874.x

Sullivan, A.M., D.M. Madison & J.R. Rohr (2003). Behavioural responses by Red-backed salamander to conspecific and heterospecific cues. Behaviour 140: 553–564.

Suter, M.J. (1944). New records of fish from Poona. Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society 44: 408–414.

Sykes, W.H. (1839). On the fishes of the Deccan. Proceedings of the General Meetings for Scientific Business of the Zoological Society of London 6: 157–165.

West-Eberhard, M.J. (2003). Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

Woody, D.R. & A. Mathis (1998). Acquired recognition of chemical stimuli from an unfamiliar predator: associative learning by adult newts, Notophthalmus viridescensCopeia 1998(4): 1027–1031; http://doi.org/10.2307/1447352

Yazdani, G.M. & A. Mahabal (1976). Fishes of Indrayani River. Biovigyanam 2: 119–121.