Journal of Threatened Taxa | www.threatenedtaxa.org | 26 August
2019 | 11(10): 14328–14333
Pollination
in an endemic and threatened monoecious herb Begonia satrapis
C.B. Clarke (Begoniaceae) in the eastern Himalaya, India
Subhankar Gurung 1, Aditya
Pradhan 2 & Arun Chettri 3
1,2,3 Taxonomy & Biodiversity Lab,
Department of Botany, Sikkim University, 6th Mile, Tadong, Gangtok, Sikkim 737102,
India.
1 subhankargurung@hotmail.com, 2
apradhan512@gmail.com, 3 achettri01@cus.ac.in (corresponding
author)
Abstract: Begonia satrapis was studied for its pollination aspects at Sumbuk, Sikkim, India.
The floral details and the foraging behaviour of insects visiting the
flowers were examined to define the pollination syndrome and its functionality
for the success of sexual reproduction in this species. The flowers do not produce nectar and offer
only pollen as floral reward to foraging insects. Therefore, male flowers were foraged more for
its pollen than the female flowers.
There was a significant difference in the visit to male and female
flowers by both Apis florea
and Bombus breviceps,
respectively. The bees spent more
time on male flowers than on female flowers.
The bees appear to rely on visual stimuli to visit male and female
flowers. The plant produces abundant
fruit and seed set in both hand and open-pollinations indicating that it is
facultatively xenogamous. The female flowers lacking
any reward resemble male flowers and in effect are pollinated by deceit.
Keywords: Apis florea, Bombus breviceps, deceit, northeastern
India, Sumbuk.
doi: https://doi.org/10.11609/jott.4256.11.10.14328-14333
Editor: A.J. Solomon Raju, Andhra University, Visakhapatnam,
India. Date of publication: 26 August
2019 (online & print)
Manuscript details: #4256 | Received 15 May 2018 |
Final received 31 July 2019 | Finally accepted 05 August 2019
Citation: Gurung, S., A. Pradhan & A.
Chettri (2019). Pollination in an endemic and threatened monoecious
herb Begonia satrapis C.B. Clarke (Begoniaceae) in the eastern Himalaya, India. Journal of Threatened Taxa 11(10): 14328–14333. https://doi.org/10.11609/jott.4256.11.10.14328-14333
Copyright: © Gurung et al. 2019. Creative Commons Attribution
4.0 International License. JoTT allows unrestricted use, reproduction, and
distribution of this article in any medium by adequate credit to the author(s)
and the source of publication.
Funding: This research has been funded by the Department of
Biotechnology (DBT), Government of India (Project No. BT/Env/BC/01/2010).
Competing interests: The authors declare no competing
interests.
Author details: Subhankar Gurung is currently doing his PhD in the Department of
Botany, Sikkim University, India. His
interest lies in studying the reproductive biology of plants with special
emphasis on understanding the role of pollinators. Aditya
Pradhan, a PhD scholar and a Junior Project Fellow (JPF), is studying
the diversity and distribution of the genus Begonia in Sikkim
Himalayas. Dr. Arun Chettri is a plant
taxonomist/ecologist who has worked extensively on plant diversity, taxonomy,
ecology and conservation of threatened plants in different forests types in northeastern India, in particular in Sikkim.
Author contribution: Study
designed by SG; Data collected by SG, AP; analysis of data done by AC, and SG
wrote the manuscript.
Acknowledgements:
The authors would like to thank
the Department of Botany, Sikkim University for providing all the logistical
support and laboratory facilities for this research. Our sincere thanks go to Dr.
M.P. Thapa, guest faculty and Sailendra Dewan,
research scholar, Department of Zoology and Dr.
Somnath Dey, Department of Zoology, Darjeeling
Government College for their assistance in helping us identify the
pollinators. Lastly, we would like to
thank the villagers for their cooperation and hospitality at Sumbuk.
INTRODUCTION
Male and female flowers provide different levels of
rewards to the pollinators. In fact,
some female flowers do not produce a pollinator reward and are actually
pollinated by deceit (Willson & Ågren 1989; Ågren & Schemske 1991).
Deceit pollination can be considered as an extreme case of unreliable
signalling in plants since flowers do not offer any reward while they benefit
from pollinator visitation (Renner 2006).
Intersexual mimicry drives the pollinators to visit the female flowers
that do not provide any reward (Little 1983; Ågren
& Schemske 1991). Mimicry
hypothesis suggests that plants that exhibit intersexual mimicry experience
selective advantage when pollinators pay more visits to reward-less female
flowers which resemble reward-providing male flowers (Ågren
& Schemske 1991).
Female flowers in many monoecious species of the genus Begonia may
attract pollinators by mimicking conspecific male flowers in which bees
mistakenly visit unrewarding female flowers (Vogel 1998; Wiens 1978; Ågren & Schemske 1991; Schemske & Ågren 1995; Corff et al. 1998).
This hypothesis is based on the remarkable similarity between
unrewarding female flowers and the pollen-rich male flowers (Ågren & Schemske 1991). Although, mimicry hypothesis and the behaviour of pollinators have been studied in some species
of Begonia (Ågren & Schemske
1991; Castillo et al. 2002; Wyatt & Sazima 2011),
no detailed study of such kind has been done in Begonia satrapis
C.B. Clarke (Begoniaceae), an IUCN Red Listed
Endangered herb of eastern Himalaya (Adhikari et al. 2018).
Little (1983) suggested that in floral mimicry
hypothesis the mimic and the model floral displays are similar, and the
pollinators mostly visit the model more often than the mimic. Accordingly, we tested this hypothesis by
studying the floral display of male and female phase inflorescences and the
pollinator behaviour in B. satrapis. The study addressed the following questions:
(i) are there any differences between the size and
morphology of male and female flowers? (ii) do pollinators discriminate between
male and female flowers? (iii) what is the success rate of intersexual mimicry
to deceive the pollinators to effect pollination?
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study site
This study was conducted in a private forest in Sumbuk which falls in the Rangit
Valley, South District, Sikkim, eastern Himalaya (27006’18.90”N
& 88022’07.32”E, altitude 555m).
The area experiences a maximum and minimum temperature of 26.90C
and 17.30C, respectively, with an annual precipitation of
2,766mm. The study site comprises a
sub-tropical type of forest where B. satrapis
flourished in abundance along the margins of this forest which is close to
human habitation. The forest
surroundings comprised species of Shorea robusta C.F. Gaertn and Schima wallichii Choisy.
Statistical data analysis
Mann-Whitney U Test was performed to evaluate the
difference in seed set rate in hand and open pollination of female
flowers. Non-parametric t-test was used
to evaluate the variation in morphological characters of male and female
flowers. Data collected from different
patches were pooled and subjected to a t-test to know whether the resulting
variation levels are statistically significant or not. A t-test was performed between the open
flowers and closed buds of both male and female flowers, respectively, to check
if there is any chronological difference in the opening of male and female
flowers.
Inflorescence sex ratio and floral morphology
A sample of 50 flowers, each for male and female sex,
was used to record floral morphometrics.
A sample of 50 plants was used to record the average number of male and
female flowers produced in individual inflorescences. Anthesis schedule and flower lifespan were
observed in the field itself.
Foraging activity and pollination
Pollinators were observed in three 3×1 m randomly
chosen patches of B. satrapis which were
reselected on 31 August, 1, 2 and 14 September 2017 (as per Ågren
& Schemske 1991).
The observation period for the day continued until the pollinators
ceased to visit the flowers. Before
making observations, number of open male and female flowers were counted within
the monitored patch. A total of 1,013
open flowers were counted in each of the inflorescences of the monitored patch
out of which 895 were male flowers and 118 were open female flowers. The foraging behaviour
of pollinators that entered the patch was observed until they moved out of
it. The number of visits to both the
male and female flower were recorded.
The time spent on each flower was recorded with a stopwatch in each
monitored patch (Male: N=50; Female: N=14).
A minimum count of 50 visits to the flowers were kept mandatory in each
monitored patch. To test if pollination
occurs by wind, several of the buds (N=25) were bagged with mosquito nets which
were made into small bags to exclude visitors in order to record the fruit and
seed set rate. Similarly, another set of
buds (N=25) were bagged and tagged with ribbons to test apomixis.
Apis florea and Bombus breviceps were the most frequent pollinators throughout
this study. A few rare visits by two
other unidentified bee species were noted.
The observations were made only on A. florea
and B. breviceps as other foragers visited
the flowers rarely.
Hand pollination
Female buds (N=50) were bagged and tagged with a coloured ribbon a day before their anthesis and were hand
pollinated on the following day. Hand
pollination was performed by brushing the fresh dehisced anther against the
stigma of a different plant. Similarly,
female buds (N=50) were tagged with a different coloured
ribbon and were left for open pollination.
The matured fruits were collected to record the number of seeds produced
against the number of ovules produced per flower. The pollen limitation was estimated as the
ratio of hand cross pollination to open pollination (Larson & Barrett
2000). The scale ranged from 0–100 where
0 indicated no pollen limitation to 100 indicating pollen limitation. All the tagged flowers could not be retrieved
at the time of their collection due to anthropogenic activities at the study
site.
RESULTS
Floral morphological details
The flowers of B. satrapis
bloom during July–October. The
flowers are open from 05.00–06.00 h. A
female flower lasts for 7–9 days while a male flower lasts for almost 15
days. The inflorescence is a cymose with male and female flowers with pink tepals (Image
1). Male flowers comprise four tepals
(2+2) while female flowers comprise five tepals (2+3). The outer tepals of male flowers were
significantly longer (1.5 ± 0.24 vs. 1.2 ± 0.17 cm; t=1.55e-09,
p<0.05) and wider (1.2 ± 0.16 vs. 1.0 ± 0.16 cm; t=4.05e-04, p<0.05) than
those of the female flowers. In addition
to the two large tepals, the inner tepals were significantly longer (1.2 ± 0.17
vs. 1.0 ± 0.18 cm; t=1.46e-06, p<0.05) than the female flowers while the
inner tepal width showed no significant difference (0.6 ± 0.17 vs. 0.7 ± 0.19;
t=0.13, p<0.05, NS) in both male and female flowers. The male flowers produced 34.9 ± 4.1 stamens
which are 4.3 ± 0.8 mm in length. The
anthers are rimose and each one produced 3,761.3 ±
1,409.4 pollen grains. The anther and
stigma of a male and female flower are yellow in colour
and are located in the centre of the flower
respectively. B. satrapis is protandrous and exhibits
temporal separation in the production of male and female flowers. The inflorescence was considered as a male
phase inflorescence when a larger number of male flowers were open compared to
the number of female flowers. The number
of open male flowers produced per inflorescence was 3.36 ± 1.84, N=50 and that
of female flowers was 0.34 ± 1.17, N=50.
The number of open male flowers and closed male flower buds per
inflorescence did not differ (t=0.39, p>0.05, NS) whereas difference was
observed between open female flowers and closed female flower buds per
inflorescence in a population (t=0.0001, p<0.05).
Foraging activity and pollination
During nine hours of observation on pollinators’ behaviour on B. satrapis a
total of 458 male and 14 female flowers were visited by A. florea and 100 male and seven female flowers were
visited by B. breviceps inside the monitored
patches (Image 1). The flowers were
foraged by bees as and when they were open and continued foraging activity
until 12.00h. Apis
florea and B. breviceps
showed strong preference to male flowers than female flowers (Table 1). The number of flower visits in the monitored
patches by A. florea varied between male (91.6
± 49.8) and female flowers (2.8 ± 2.2; t=0.007, p<0.05). Similarly, 107 flowers visited by B. breviceps varied between male (20 ± 8.3) and female
flowers (1.4 ± 1.3; t= 0.005, p<0.05). A. florea and
B. breviceps spent more time on male flowers
than on female flowers due to pollen collection activity (Table 2). Apis florea used their legs to remove the pollen while B.
breviceps performed vibration to collect pollen. Both bee species discriminated female flowers
after making first visit to them. The
foraging activity of A. florea was slower than
B. breviceps while there was no significant
difference in time spent on male flowers (t=0.31, p>0.05, NS) and female
flowers(t=0.13, p>0.05, NS) by both bee species.
Hand pollination
Although fruit set was observed in both hand
pollinated and open pollinated flowers, hand pollination between male and
female flowers of different plants resulted in an increase in seed set (95.4 ±
8.1, N=17) than open pollination (81.1 ± 17.2, N=17) and showed a significant
difference between the two (U=0.001, p<0.05). Fruit and seed set was absent in both wind
pollination and apomixis. The estimated
pollen limitation (1.17) indicated that the pollinators deposit adequate pollen
in its natural environment.
DISCUSSION
B. satrapis
is a monoecious species with both
male and female flowers borne in the same inflorescence whose morphological
characters varied significantly between male and female flowers in a
population. The larger tepals of male
flowers appear to be an adaptation to provide visual stimuli to pollinator bees
to locate the flowers that provide the reward (Ågren
& Schemske 1991).
The anthesis of male flowers prior to female flowers enable the
pollinators to habituate themselves to the forage source and visit rewardless female flowers by deceit when available on the
same or different inflorescences of the same or different conspecific
plants. Similar observations were
reported in Jacaratia dolichaula
(Bawa 1980) where pollinators first encountered
the rewarding male flowers and therefore, reduced the chances of discrimination
of early flowering rewardless female flowers (Corff et al. 1998).
The stigmas of female flowers are yellow and strongly resemble male
flowers. The female flowers attract
pollinators by mimicking male flowers (Wiens 1978; Ågren
& Schemske 1991).
The rimose anthers are grouped in large number
which facilitate pollen collection by vibration (Wyatt & Sazima 2011). The
two important foragers A. florea and B. breviceps showed more preference to the male flowers
than the female flowers. This possibly
could be because of the pollinators ability to recognize unrewarding flowers
(Wyatt & Sazima 2011). It was observed that A. florea seemed diffident to collect pollen from a male
flower immediately after its visit to a male flower. The lower rate of visitation and the
hesitation shown in collecting pollen could be because of the lack of fragrance
which has been experimentally proven to be an important aspect to encourage
landing on a female flower (Lunau 1991; Schemske & Ågren 1995). Despite the negligible visits to a female
flower a high seed set was observed in flowers left for open pollination which
could be because of pollination by vibration (Wyatt & Sazima
2011). When a pollinator performs vibration while it visits a female flower by
deception it transfers a large load of small and powdery pollen to the stigma
(Wyatt & Sazima 2011). A single visit is adequate to deposit a large
amount of pollen load to the stigma (Wyatt & Sazima
2011). Seed set in hand pollination was
significantly higher than that of open pollination. The lack of pollen limitation, however,
indicates that the pollinators are efficient in depositing pollen for
successful seed set. Since wind pollination
and apomixis are absent in B. satrapis, it
becomes evident that it depends largely on pollinators for its successful
sexual reproduction.
CONCLUSION
It is apparent from our study that the pollinators
prefer to visit the polleniferous male flowers more
than the rewardless female flowers. The imperfect discrimination by the
pollinators by the number of “mistaken” visits to female flowers, however,
guarantees pollination by deceit.
Further, abundant fruit and seed set indicate that pollinators are
efficient in contributing to the production of fruit and seed in B. satrapis. The study indicates that A. florea and B. breviceps are
the principal pollinators of B. satrapis and
hence, this plant is melittophilous.
Table 1. Number of A. florea
and B. breviceps visits to male and female
flowers of B. satrapis.
|
Number
of flowers in a patch |
Number
of flowers visited |
||||
|
|
|
A. florea |
A. breviceps |
||
Patch |
Male |
Female |
Male |
Female |
Male |
Female |
1 |
177 |
7 |
60 |
2 |
22 |
2 |
1 |
177 |
7 |
175 |
6 |
32 |
0 |
2 |
173 |
26 |
48 |
2 |
15 |
2 |
3 |
197 |
16 |
83 |
4 |
10 |
3 |
2 |
173 |
63 |
92 |
0 |
21 |
0 |
Table 2. Time (in seconds) spent on male and female
flowers of B. satrapis by A. florea and B. breviceps.
|
A. florea |
B. breviceps |
||
|
Male |
Female |
Male |
Female |
Patch |
Mean ± SD |
Mean ± SD |
Mean ± SD |
Mean ± SD |
1 |
4.9
± 3.4 |
0.7
± 0.3 |
1.1 ± 0.6 |
1.4 ± 0.1 |
1 |
4.6
± 2.9 |
0.4
± 0.1 |
1.2 ± 0.7 |
- |
2 |
3.1
± 2.4 |
0.6
± 0.3 |
1.9 ± 1.2 |
1.0 ± 0.2 |
3 |
5.4
± 3.2 |
0.7
± 0.6 |
2.0 ± 1.6 |
1.0 ± 0.1 |
2 |
2.5
± 1.4 |
- |
1.4 ± 0.7 |
- |
|
4.1
± 2.7 |
0.6
± 0.3 |
1.5 ± 0.9 |
1.2 ± 0.1 |
Note: The visits to flowers outside the patch were
also considered for better results.
For
figure & image – click here
REFERENCES
Adhikari, D., Z. Reshi, B.K. Datta, A. Chettri, K.
Upadhaya, M.A. Shah, P.P. Singh, R. Tiwary, K. Majumdar, A. Pradhan, M.L. Thakur, N. Salam,
S.H. Mir, Z.A. Kaloo & S.K. Barik (2018). Inventory and characterization of new populations
through ecological niche modelling improve threat assessment. Current
Science 114(3): 519–531. https://doi.org/10.18520/cs/v114/i03/519-531
Ågren, J. & D.W. Schemske
(1991). Pollination by deceit in a
neotropical monoeious herb, Begonia involucrata. Biotropica 23:
235–241. https://doi.org/10.2307/2388200
Bawa, K.S. (1980). Mimicry of male by female flowers and intrasexual
competition for pollinators in Jacaratia dolichaula (D. Smith) Woodson (Caricaceae).
Evolution 34: 467–474. https://doi.org/10.2307/2408216
Castillo, R.A., C. Cordero &
C.A. Domínguez (2002). Are reward
polymorphisms subject to frequency- and density-dependent selection? Evidence
from a monoecious species pollinated by deceit. Journal of Evolutionary
Biology 15: 544–552. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1420-9101.2002.00425.x
Corff, J.L., J. Ågren & D.W. Schemske (1998). Floral display, pollinator discrimination and female
reproductive success in two monoecious Begonia species. Ecology 79:
1610–1619.
Larson, B.M.H & S.C.H. Barrett
(2000). A comparative analysis of pollen
limitation in flowering plants. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 69: 503–520.
Little, R.J. (1983). A review of floral food deception mimicries with
comments on floral mutualism, pp294–309. In: Jones, C.E. & R.J. Little
(eds.). Handbook of Experimental Pollination Biology. New York Press,
New York, 558pp.
Lunau, K. (1991). Innate flower recognition in bumblebees (Bombusterrestris, B. lucorum;
Apidae): Optical signals from stamens as landing
reaction releasers. Ethology 88: 203–214. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1991.tb00275.x
Renner, S.S. (2006). Rewardless flowers in Angiosperms and the role of insect
cognition in their evolution, pp123–144. In: Waser,
N.M. & J. Ollerton (eds.). Plant-pollinator Interactions. From
Specialization to Generalization. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Schemske, D.W. & J. Ågren
(1995). Deceit pollination and selection
on female flower size in Begonia involucrata:
an experimental approach. Evolution 49: 207–214. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1995.tb05972.x
Vogel, S. (1998). Remarkable nectarines: structure, ecology, organophyletic perspectives IV. Miscellaneous cases. Flora
193: 225–248.
Wiens, D. (1978). Mimicry in plants, pp365–403. In: Hecht, M.K., W.C. Steere & B. Wallace (eds.). Evolutionary Biology.
Plenum Press, New York, 655pp.
Willson, M.F. & J. Ågren
(1989). Differential floral rewards and
pollination by deceit in unisexual flowers. Oikos 55: 23–29. http://doi.org/10.2307/3565868
Wyatt, G.E & M. Sazima (2011). Pollination and reproductive biology of thirteen
species of Begonia in the Serra Do Mar State Park, Sao Pãulo, Brazil. Journal of Pollination Ecology 6: 95–107.